Three-dimensional reconstruction of neuronal morphology has been

Three-dimensional reconstruction of neuronal morphology has been an established and widespread laboratory technique for three decades (Halavi et al., 2012), but recent progress in neurobiology, microscopy, and information technology has expanded both the breadth and the depth of these studies. We can now selectively label various neuron types, confirming their stunning phenotypic diversity and allowing identification of their distinguishing properties (Ascoli et al., 2008). Advancements in light microscopy are increasing the resolution,

contrast, speed, and applicability of neuronal imaging, revealing more refined selleckchem and previously inaccessible morphological details. Continuous increase of computational power and algorithmic sophistication are constantly adding to the available applications of data processing. Cell labeling and

tract tracing have long been pursued to elucidate the complex neuronal network architecture. Different staining methods developed over the years have yielded a rich histological toolbox (Figure 2A). Certain techniques are better suited for specific experiments and preparations, and selecting the appropriate method is crucial. Basic criteria include selleck compound clear contrast between the neurite and background tissue and maximum labeling extent of the neuronal arbor. Here, we overview a selection of labeling approaches (for more comprehensive coverage of these topics, please see Köbbert et al., 2000; Lanciego and Wouterlood, 2011). Bulk dye loading is used to visualize the gross morphology and connectivity patterns of neurons, which can then be traced individually or as networks. The following is a out selection of common dyes employed in morphological

studies. Horseradish peroxidase (HRP) is visualized by histochemical analysis and its sensitivity is enhanced by conjugation with a nontoxic fragment of cholera toxin or with wheat germ agglutinin (Trojanowski et al., 1982), which slows removal from the loaded neurons and allows for visualization of the full structure. The dextran amine is conjugated to a fluorescent dye and is detected by peroxidase and 3, 3′-diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride (DAB) reaction. The reaction product is distributed homogenously and fills the entire neuronal structure (Reiner et al., 2000). Phaseolus vulgaris Leucoagglutinin (PHA-L) is an anterograde tracer with unknown receptor-based uptake mechanism. Using antibodies against the lectin, PHA-L staining can be detected in the entire neuronal structure, including axon collaterals and terminals. The bleach-resistant properties of Fluoro-Gold (hydroxystilbamidine), an unconjugated fluorescent dye, make it a “gold standard” in labeling.

The threshold-quadratic nonlinearity appears to be a general prop

The threshold-quadratic nonlinearity appears to be a general property of signal integration in the recorded ganglion cells and presumably corresponds to the nonlinear processing that had been suggested to underlie Afatinib several

specific visual functions solved by the retina (Ölveczky et al., 2003, Gollisch and Meister, 2008, Gollisch and Meister, 2010 and Münch et al., 2009). Thresholding has been considered previously to lead to nonlinear receptive fields (Shapley and Victor, 1979, Victor and Shapley, 1979, Demb et al., 2001, Ölveczky et al., 2003, Geffen et al., 2007, Gollisch and Meister, 2008 and Münch et al., 2009), though often a threshold-linear operation has been hypothesized, rather than the threshold-quadratic transformation

observed in this study. Consistent with these previous findings, the source of this nonlinearity appears to be the bipolar cell input into the ganglion cell; the spatial scale of the nonlinearities GSK1210151A fits the receptive field size of bipolar cells (Figure 4), and this type of nonlinearity is not affected by a block of inhibitory neurotransmission (Figure 7). The threshold-quadratic nonlinearity may arise in the voltage response of individual bipolar cells (Burkhardt and Fahey, 1998) or in the synaptic transmission at the bipolar cell terminals (Baccus et al., 2008 and Molnar et al., 2009). It is noteworthy that iso-latency curves were more consistent in their shapes and always clearly displayed the quadratic part of the nonlinearity (Figure 3G), whereas iso-rate curves, even for cells that were not classified as homogeneity detectors, sometimes showed a tendency toward more linear integration (Figure 3H, see also Figure 3B for an example). This may be explained by local adaptation, for example, synaptic depression, which somewhat reduces the efficiency of strong local stimulation during the course of the spike burst. It is further interesting to note that no found linearly integrating ganglion cells were observed in our study.

This might be a feature of the investigated species; in the cat retina, for example, X-type cells would be predicted to have iso-response curves in the shape of straight lines. The particular sensitivity to homogeneous illumination of the receptive field in homogeneity detectors appears to arise from inhibitory interactions in the circuit. The nonconvex shape of the iso-rate curves was always abolished by removal of inhibition from the retinal circuitry, including experiments with reduced stimulus area so that different ranges of input into the system were tested. Otherwise, the nonconvex shape proved robust to changes in stimulus layout and overall activation level. Together with the success of the computational inhibition model, this supports a principal role of inhibition for generating the response features of homogeneity detectors.

They then recorded the activity of individual cells in the face p

They then recorded the activity of individual cells in the face patches in response to the artificial faces and found that the cells do indeed respond to contrasts between facial features. Ohayon and his colleagues later studied the cells’ response to images of real faces and found that, again, responses increased with the number of contrast-defined features. Tsao, Freiwald, and their colleagues had found earlier that cells in the face patches respond selectively to the shape of

some facial features, such as noses and eyes (Tsao et al., 2008). Ohayon’s findings now showed that this selective response depends on luminance relative to other parts of the face. Most of the cells they studied respond both click here to contrast and to the shape of facial features, which leads us to an important conclusion: contrast is useful for face detection, and shape is useful for face recognition. These studies have shed new light on the nature of the templates the brain uses to detect faces. Behavioral studies suggest a powerful link between the brain’s face detection machinery and the areas 5-Fluoracil manufacturer of the brain that control attention, which may account for why faces—and particularly portraits—draw our attention so strongly. When psychoanalysis emerged from Vienna early in the twentieth century, it represented a revolutionary way of thinking about the human mind and its disorders. The excitement

surrounding the theory of unconscious mental processes increased as psychoanalysis was brought

to the United States by immigrants from Germany and Austria. Under the influence of psychoanalysis, psychiatry was transformed in the decades following World War II from an experimental medical discipline closely related to neurology into a nonempirical specialty focused on psychotherapy. In the 1950s academic psychiatry abandoned some of its roots in biology and experimental medicine and gradually became a therapeutic discipline based on psychoanalytic theory. Over the next 50 years, psychoanalysis exhausted much of its novel investigative power. It also failed to submit its assumptions to the sort of rigorous tests that are needed to inspire confidence. Indeed, it was far better at generating ideas than at testing them. Fortunately, some people in the psychoanalytic community thought that Edoxaban empirical research was essential to the future of the discipline. Because of them, two trends have gained momentum in the last several decades. One is the insistence on evidence-based psychotherapy; the other is an effort to align psychoanalysis with the emerging biology of mind. Perhaps the most important driving force for evidence-based therapy has been Aaron Beck, a psychoanalyst at the University of Pennsylvania. Whereas traditional psychoanalysis teaches that mental problems arise from unconscious conflicts, Beck became convinced that conscious thought processes also play a role in mental disorders.

A defective midline glial scaffold is in part responsible for the

A defective midline glial scaffold is in part responsible for the erroneous ipsilateral projection of RGCs in zebrafish belladona/lhx2 mutants ( Seth et al., 2006). We therefore analyzed sections through the optic chiasm of Nrp1 null mutants with two established markers for midline glia, RC2 and NrCAM ( Marcus et al., 1995 and Williams et al., 2006). However, there were no obvious differences in the arrangement of the RC2-positive glia ( Figure 2E), and NrCAM was still expressed by these cells

( Figure S2B). The CD44/SSEA-positive neurons at the posterior border of the developing optic chiasm, which are required for RGC axon extension across the midline ( Marcus et al., 1995 and Sretavan et al., 1995), were also present in Nrp1 null mutants ( Figure S2C). Finally, we looked at the expression of the ephrin B2 gene (Efnb2; ephrin-B2), which encodes the guidance cue that repels EPHB1-expressing RGC axons from the midline Autophagy Compound Library manufacturer to steer them into the ipsilateral path ( Williams et al., 2003). However, ephrin B2 expression at the chiasmatic midline was similar in mutants and wild-types ( Figure 2E). We conclude that the architecture of the optic chiasm is not obviously perturbed in

Nrp1 null mutants. We next asked if the increased ipsilateral projection in Nrp1 null mutants was due CP-868596 solubility dmso to an enlargement of the retinal domain that gives rise to ipsilaterally projecting RGCs. These neurons arise in two overlapping phases in the mouse. An early but transient

ipsilateral projection arises from RGCs in the dorsocentral retina between E12.5 and E14.5; subsequently, RGCs located predominantly in the ventrotemporal retina establish the permanent ipsilateral projection between E14.5 and E16.5 ( Godement et al., 1987, Williams et al., 2003 and Williams et al., 2006). Consistent with previous studies, Ephb1 was expressed in the E14.5 wild-type dorsocentral retina, where the RGCs forming the early ipsilateral heptaminol projection arise ( Figure 2F). This expression domain appeared similar in Nrp1 null mutants ( Figure 2F). Due to lethality at E15.5, we were not able to examine Ephb1 expression in RGCs forming the permanent ipsilateral projection in Nrp1 null mutants. ZIC2 is a transcription factor that is both necessary and sufficient to specify the permanent ipsilateral RGCs and is expressed prior to Ephb1 in these cells and by undifferentiated cells in the ciliary margin ( Figure 2F; see Herrera et al., 2003 and Tian et al., 2008). Importantly, the Zic2 expression pattern was similar in Nrp1 null mutants and controls, with no expansion of the normal expression domain within the RGC layer or ectopic expression by RGCs in other regions of the retina ( Figure 2F). We conclude that NRP1 signaling does not regulate chiasm development by affecting the specification of RGCs that give rise to the transient or permanent ipsilateral projections.

While the sRPE and sAPE were generated with the simulated-other’s

While the sRPE and sAPE were generated with the simulated-other’s reward and choice

probability, respectively, this choice probability was generated in each trial selleck screening library by using the reward probability. Altogether, we propose that the sAPE is a general, critical component for simulation learning. The sAPE provides an additional, but also “natural,” learning signal that could arise from simulation by direct recruitment, as it was readily generated from the simulated-other’s choice probability given the subject’s observation of the other’s choices. This error should be useful for refining the learning of the other’s hidden variables, particularly if the other behaves differently from the way one would

expect for oneself, i.e., the prediction made by direct recruitment simulation (Mitchell et al., 2006). As such, we consider this error and the associated pattern of neural activation to be an accessory signal to the core simulation process of valuation occurring in the vmPFC, which further http://www.selleckchem.com/products/ly2157299.html suggests a more general hierarchy of learning signals in simulation apart from and beyond the sAPE. As the other’s choice behavior in this study was only related to a specific personality or psychological isotype, being risk neutral, it will be interesting to see whether and how the sAPE is modified to facilitate learning about the other depending on different personality or psychological isotypes of the other. Also, in this study, because we chose to investigate the ADP ribosylation factor sAPE as a general signal, learning about the nature of the other’s risk behavior or risk parameters in our model was treated as secondary, being fixed in all trials. However, subjects might have learned the other’s risk parameter and/or adjusted their own risk parameter over the course of the trials. How these types of learning complement simulation learning examined in the present study shown here will require further investigation. Together, we demonstrate that simulation requires distinct prefrontal circuits to learn the

other’s valuation process by direct recruitment and to refine the overall learning trajectory by tracking the other’s behavioral variation. Because our approach used a fundamental form of simulation learning, we expect that our findings may be broadly relevant to modeling and predicting the behavior of others in many domains of cognition, including higher level mentalizing in more complex tasks involving social interactions, recursive reasoning, and/or different task goals. We propose that the signals and computations underlying higher level mentalizing in complex social interactions might be built upon those identified in the present study. It remains to be determined how the simulated-other’s reward and action prediction error signals are utilized and modified when task complexity is increased.

, 2011) We also found that VP excitatory effects on PVN-RVLM neu

, 2011). We also found that VP excitatory effects on PVN-RVLM neurons were blocked by flufenamic acid (FFA; 200 μM) (n = 9; Figure S2F), a relatively specific blocker of TRPM4/TRPM5 channels (Ullrich et al., 2005). In the presence of FFA, PVN-RVLM neurons were still capable of displaying a burst of action potentials in response to a puff of 20 μM NMDA (n = 3; data not shown), indicating that FFA effects were not due to nonspecific effects on overall neuronal function or due to changes in PVN-RVLM responsiveness

to NMDA. Further studies, however, are needed to precisely identify the molecular identity of the CAN channel underlying VP actions in presympathetic neurons. Selleck LY2835219 To directly probe for a crosstalk between MNNs and presympathetic neurons, we developed an approach using transgenic EGFP-VP rats

(Ueta et al., 2005) that received an injection of a fluorescent retrograde tracer in the RVLM (Figure S3). Our approach consisted of selectively http://www.selleckchem.com/products/crenolanib-cp-868596.html activating individual VP neurons using laser photolysis of caged NMDA while simultaneously monitoring the electrical activity of neighboring presympathetic neurons in acute hypothalamic slices. To validate this approach, we show that laser photolysis of caged NMDA onto restricted somatodendritic regions of patched EGFP-VP neurosecretory neurons induced reproducible inward currents along with a concurrent high-frequency burst of action potentials (Figure 4A), previously shown to efficiently evoke dendritic release of peptides from MNNs in brain slices (Kombian et al., 1997). Moreover, photolysis of caged NMDA in the somata of Fluo-5F-loaded EGFP-VP neurons increased [Ca2+]i levels, which rapidly propagated into dendritic compartments (Figure S4). To test the hypothesis that dendritic VP release acts as a crosstalk signal between neurosecretory and presympathetic neurons, we then obtained patch recordings from PVN-RVLM neurons and assessed their responses to photolysis

of caged NMDA in neighboring EGFP-VP neurons. On average, three different EGFP-VP neurons were photoactivated per patched PVN-RVLM neuron. The mean distance between the somata of presympathetic and the photoactivated VP neurons was 111.6 ± 7.9 μm. Photolysis of caged NMDA at the somata of individual Mephenoxalone EGFP-VP neurons consistently evoked an excitatory response in neighboring PVN-RVLM neurons, characterized by a burst of activity, which was underlain by a membrane depolarization (n = 38 EGFP-VP neurons/11 PVN-RVLM neurons, p < 0.001; Figure 4B). Responses in presympathetic neurons occurred with a mean latency of 3.5 ± 1.0 s following photolysis in neighboring EGFP-VP neurons. In a few cases (n = 4), stimulation of an EGFP-VP neuron failed to evoke a response in PVN-RVLM neurons, which were, however, responsive to other EGFP-VP neurons in the same preparation. Direct photolysis of caged NMDA onto the recorded neurons (EGFP-VP or presympathetic) resulted in an almost instantaneous effect (p < 0.

If the amplitude had a negative value with respect to the baselin

If the amplitude had a negative value with respect to the baseline, that site was added to the abduction map. In the case of bidirectional movement profiles where both the positive and negative components satisfied the amplitude criteria, the corresponding site was included in both the abduction and adduction maps and counted as overlap between maps. For each map, the center of gravity was calculated along with the mean amplitude and latency for the nine pixels closest to the center point. Maps with mean amplitude of <0.1 mm at the center were excluded from further analysis.

Separation between Mab and Mad was defined as the distance between the centers of gravity for each map. After completing two to five INCB024360 research buy motor maps, mice were raised into a sitting posture with their forelimbs hanging freely. Stimulus sites were placed as close to the centers of the abduction and adduction representations as possible without targeting major blood vessels, since these absorb light strongly (Ayling et al., 2009).

Fifty-one frames were captured at a rate of 100 Hz beginning 10 ms prior to laser stimulus onset, and paw trajectories were generated from the raw image sequences using the plugin “MTrack2” for ImageJ. Ten to 20 repetitions were then averaged for each trial, and speed and angle profiles were calculated based on this average trajectory. ChR2 transgenic mice were implanted with optical fibers (Thorlabs Apoptosis Compound Library datasheet BFH48-200) extending to the cortical surface and terminating in a ferrule connector (Precision Fiber Products) fixed to the skull with dental acrylic and bone screws. Two fibers were implanted, targeted to the mean coordinates of the Mab and Mad map centers. These locations were stimulated alternately (5 mW 5 ms pulses at 100 Hz for 500 ms) using a 473 nm laser (IKECOOL IKE-473-100-OP) connected via an optical commutator (Doric). Stimulus evoked behavior was recorded by a CCD camera

(Dalsa 1M60) and frame grabber (EPIX). Limb trajectories were analyzed in the same manner as the anesthetized data, except that paw position was tracked using the plugin “Manual Tracking” for ImageJ. Glass pipets (tip width 10–20 μm) containing a 0.25 mm bare silver wire were filled with 1% fast green PDK4 in 3 M sodium chloride. A micromanipulator (Sutter) was used to advance the pipet to a depth of 700 μm. Stimulation sites were matched with those targeted by laser stimulation in the same animals. Trains of 200 μs 100 μA pulses at 200 Hz with 10–500 ms durations were generated by an AM systems stimulator and a WPI stimulus isolator. For motor mapping experiments involving virally transduced mice, 1–2 μl of adeno-associated virus (serotype 2/1 CAG-ChR2-GFP) was injected through a burr hole into the sensorimotor cortex of ChR2-negative mice 2 mm lateral of bregma at a depth of 500 μm using a 5 μl Hamilton syringe with a 33 gauge needle and a syringe pump (WPI). Mice recovered for 2–4 weeks before being used in experiments.

All procedures were done at 4°C ECS was induced through a consta

All procedures were done at 4°C. ECS was induced through a constant-current generator (ECT unit; Ugo Basile, Comerio, Italy) (Cole et al., 1990), in accordance with the guidelines see more of the Johns Hopkins Animal Care and Use Committee. The brain was dissected 2 hr after ECS and placed immediately into cold (2.5°C) modified CSF composed of the following (in mM): 110 choline chloride, 2.5 KCl, 7 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 2.4 Na-pyruvate, 1.3 Na-ascorbic acid, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, and 20 glucose. Coronal brain slices of the prefrontal cortex (250 μm)

were prepared from P20-22 WT and Homer1a KO mice using a Vibratome 3000 (Leica Biosystems, St. Louis LLC, St. Louis, MO). After cutting, slices were incubated for 15 min at 32°C and then for up to 3 hr at 25°C in ACSF. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from cortical cultures and slices were carried out at 30°C–32°C. Pyramidal neurons in cortical cultures and the layer II-III region of the prefrontal cortex were visually identified using Dodt Gradient Contrast.

Transfected neurons were also visually identified under epifluorescence. learn more The recording chamber was continuously perfused with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM): 124 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.3 MgCl2, 2.5 CaCl2, 1 NaH2PO4, 26.2 NaHCO3, and 10 glucose, equilibrated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 (pH 7.4, 305 ± 5 mmol/kg). The bath solution also contained both 1 μM TTX and 10 μM GABAzine to block action potential dependent EPSCs and GABAA receptors, respectively. The pipette solution contained (in mM): 90 Cs-methansulfonate, 48.5 CsCl, 5 ethylene glycol tetraacetic acid,

2 MgCl2, 2 Na-ATP, 0.4 Na-GTP, and 5 HEPES (pH 7.2, 290 ± 2 mmol/kg). Patch pipettes were pulled from borosilicate glass (4–5 MΩ) using a horizontal puller (Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA). Signals were recorded with a Multiclamp 700B (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA) amplifier, filtered at 2 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz. To detect a sufficient number of events (200 events per neuron), recordings were performed on gap 17-DMAG (Alvespimycin) HCl free mode (sweeps of 30 s without any latency). Data were acquired 3 min after achieving the whole-cell configuration. Series resistances (Rs) of recordings ranged between 10 and 15 MΩ. Cells were rejected from analysis if Rs changed by more than 15%. mEPSCs were analyzed by Mini Analysis Software (Synaptosoft, NJ). All group data are shown as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Statistical comparison was performed by the independent t test, ANOVA for multiple comparison (see Figures 1F and 5G), or Fisher’s exact test (see Figure 7F). All drugs were purchased from Tocris (Ellisville, MO) except for TTX (Ascent Scientific LLC, Princeton, NJ). All the data were analyzed by two-tailed Student’s t test except the analysis of the multiple comparisons (Figures 1F and 5G). Error bars indicate the SEM. We thank Dr. Alison Barth of Carnegie Mellon University for Fos-GFP mice.

To investigate how humans learn correlations between

outc

To investigate how humans learn correlations between

outcomes we scanned 16 subjects using fMRI while they performed a “resource management” game. This task invoked a scenario whereby a power company generates fluctuating amounts of electricity from two renewable energy sources, a solar plant and a wind park. We instructed subjects to create an energy portfolio under a specific goal constraint necessitating keeping the total energy output as constant as possible (Figure 1A). Subjects accomplished this by adjusting weights that determined how the two resources were linearly combined. A normative best DAPT chemical structure performance is achievable by finding a solution that exploits knowledge of the covariance structure of these resources (Figure 1B), a task design that approximates a simple portfolio problem in finance. Importantly, the outcomes of the two resources covaried with each other and this correlation between the two outcomes changed probabilistically over time, requiring subjects to continuously update their estimate of the current

correlation structure. This task is well suited for assessing subjects’ estimate of the correlation strength because a good performance is only accomplished if subjects learn both the distribution of returns for each resource as well as their correlation. We rewarded participants according to how stable they kept the total output of their mixed energy learn more portfolio relative to the variance resulting from an optimal strategy (specified by MPT-calculated optimal weights). We speculated that subjects might solve the task by learning the correlative strength between the resources via a correlation prediction error, calculated from the cross-product of the individual resources’ outcome prediction errors (Figure 1C). This envisages that subjects represent a continuous measure of outcome correlation and update this metric on a trial-by-trial basis. To rule out alternative strategies we examined other computational models that could

not be used to guide choice in our task, and fitted the free parameters of each model to get model predicted portfolio weights that most closely resembled the actual responses for each subject. One such alternative model-based strategy is to exploit trial-by-trial evidence to update a representation of the portfolio weights directly instead of first estimating the correlation coefficient. Similar to correlation learning, this model makes assumptions about the structure of the task and uses individual resource outcomes as a basis for learning. The main difference between the covariance based model and this model is that in the former, subjects update an estimate of the correlation via a prediction error and then translate this correlation strength into task-specific weights on every trial, whereas in the latter the estimates of task-dependent weights (i.e.

To

To selleck chemicals characterize the behavioral learning of visuomotor associations in both species, we used a logistic regression algorithm (Smith et al., 2004) to generate learning curves based on binary responses (Law et al., 2005 and Wirth et al., 2003). Typical learning curves consisted of a variable number of predominantly incorrect responses, followed by a sharp transition to predominantly correct responses. Associations were considered learned once the lower bound 95% confidence interval of the logistic regression became greater than would be expected by chance. The trial on which the learning passed this criterion was considered the

“learning trial.” An analysis of the learning trial indicated that the curves initially presented within a set could be ordered, identifying “fast,” “medium,” and “slow” learned conditions, a pattern observed both in monkeys (F(3,21) = 17.92; p < 0.001) and humans (F(3,87) = 34.91; p < 0.0005) that was linear in nature (F(1,36) = 115.97; p < 0.0005). A similar analysis of the maximum learning curve slopes reinforced the idea that the Ku-0059436 research buy curves could be ordered linearly (F(1,36) = 52.45; p < 0.0005). Overall, the pattern suggests that a common strategy was adopted by both monkeys

and humans during which only one association was “worked on” at a time (Hadj-Bouziane and Boussaoud, 2003). Although the overall learning strategy appeared remarkably similar across species, not surprisingly, both the speed of learning and number of learned associations were superior in humans compared to monkeys. Human subjects had steeper learning curves than monkeys, as evidenced by differences in the average maximum slope of learned visuomotor associations (t(125) = 13.81; p < 0.0001) and a smaller number trials to criterion (Humans: mean 4.67, range 2–28, SEM

0.68; Monkeys: mean 17.14, range 2–39, SEM 0.69; t(30) = 5.483; p < 0.0001). As a consequence, humans learned significantly more associations per session than monkeys (monkeys = 1.73, humans = 20.26; t(30) = 13.64; p < 0.0001). Of the 152 visuomotor associations presented during the 74 recording sessions, monkeys learned a total of 56.56% (86) associations. Conversely, of the 924 stimulus-location associations presented in 31 scanning sessions, TCL human subjects learned a total of 67.96% (628) associations. Thus, overall, humans also learned a significantly greater percentage of conditions than did the monkeys (χ2(1) = 7.58; p < 0.01). To identify homologies between the neurophysiological responses in the monkeys and human hippocampus and entorhinal cortex during the performance of the same behavioral task, we measured LFP recordings from two monkeys (Figures 1C and 1D) and BOLD fMRI from 31 human subjects focused on these two regions (Goense and Logothetis, 2008, Kirwan et al., 2007, Law et al., 2005 and Logothetis, 2002).